Companion to Indicators of TMS paper

One of my interests that has persisted throughout my PhD was on the measurement strategies for transactive memory systems (TMS). I have a paper now available in advance at Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice that describes one process I have gone through over the last several years to assess and refine our assessment strategies for TMS. This post will be a companion to that paper where I’ll be sharing some tips and my code to do similar assessments of TMS in your own work.

So what is TMS? A transactive memory system is colloquially called the ‘knowledge of who knows what in a team.’ It is the characteristic of teams whereby they can implicitly coordinate their work because it is clear who is good at what. If we zoom in on the individuals, a TMS contains a few layers. I know what I know (though the accuracy of that knowledge is always debatable, see Dunning Kruger effect). And I know what my teammates know. I may learn about my teammate’s knowledge in a few ways: I guess based on titles or stereotypes (see Yoon and Hollingshead, 2010), they tell me, or I see knowledge demonstrated. A team can take advantage of a TMS to improve their work if we all have a mental map of our own and other’s knowledge.

If we look at the way that TMS is assessed, however, typically we look at the behavioral indicators that a team has developed a TMS: specialization, credibility, and coordination. Other work has looked at the creation and use of knowledge maps to assess TMS (Austin, 2003). I wanted to determine to what extent these different ways of assessing a TMS capture something similar or different. You can read the full paper to see the full story.

For the rest of this post, I want to describe how I assessed the Knowledge and Meta-Knowledge indicators of TMS. These are the steps you would also want to follow in any of your own attempts to do the same.

Step 1: The survey

Lewis’s (2003) measure of TMS is the most common currently in use. This survey contains 15 Likert-style questions asking about the extent to which the group has specialization, credibility, and coordination. I encourage authors to continue using this scale though, importantly, the Coordination sub-scale appears to do the heavy-lifting in terms of predicting performance in my paper and most of the studies I have run. This is evidently not always the case, see Bachrach et al. (2018) for a meta-analysis. As you will likely already be including a survey, measuring knowledge and meta-knowledge should be pretty easy to add on.

First, determine your expertise areas. Austin (2003) describes a process to determine what are the relevant areas of expertise in field settings. I was doing lab studies, so I chose the main knowledge elements of work elements of my task as expertise areas. There was only one person with information about the “filter” component of the graphical programming task, for example, so I asked a question about knowledge of the “filter.” I had multiple tasks or areas of work in Studies 1 and 3 so I captured knowledge in both areas. You’ll want to use factor analysis to confirm 2 areas of knowledge and then you’ll have to handle some calculations differently later on.

There are a few options in terms of wording your survey questions. In study 1 and 3, I asked a question for each expertise area: “For the statements below, rate each member below on how much you agree or disagree that that statement describes the member. 1. Knows what the Filter module does” I then had the participants respond. If you are only measuring Knowledge, just have one question on each . If you also want to capture meta-knowledge, you might structure your question this way as a matrix:

Example of Knowledge/Meta-knowledge question from Study 2

Example of Knowledge/Meta-knowledge question from Study 2

 I encourage you to give the opportunity to select “Don’t know” and I’ll show you how to deal with that later.

For Study 2 in the paper, I structured this question differently with a question per participant and then a matrix for each expertise area:

Example of question posed to 1 member on the Knowledge in Study 3

Example of question posed to 1 member on the Knowledge in Study 3

Again, you’d only have 1 question like this if you want to only measure knowledge or you would repeat this question once for each member to capture meta-knowledge.

I did see some small differences in the effects of the underlying calculated variables between Study 2 and 3, so it is possible that this difference in presentation may have driven some of those difference. I don’t think either presentation is superior so I encourage other authors to use the presentation they prefer.

Step 2: Data Structure

Depending on what question-type you used, whether you captured Meta-knowledge, etc. your data may be in a different structure. Be sure you get it in a structure where Group, Member, and Expertise Categories are each variables.

IF YOU ASKED ABOUT META KNOWLEDGE

It is easiest if you have a different variable for each expertise-area by person. So, in Group 10, Member A, they may have a column for each member who rated them on each expertise area. If you have only 2 expertise areas for example, you’d have these columns: “Expertise1_A, Expertise2_A, Expertise1_B, Expertise2_B, etc.” I encourage you to have these columns grouped by member. Create a new set of columns which is for the member’s self-rating. IF YOU ARE MEASURING ACCURACY, create a column for the average of each member’s alter rated expertise scores for each category at the group level. That’s what I did and I think it de-complicates some things later on. Thus you’d have the following columns from the above example: “Expertise1_A, Expertise2_A, Expertise1_B, Expertise2_B, Expertise1_Self, Expertise2_Self, Expertise1_A_Avg, Expertise2_A_Avg, Expertise1_B_Group, Expertise2_B_Group”

Step 3: Data Cleaning

Once you have your data, we can start cleaning things up.

IF YOU ALLOWED FOR “DON’T KNOW”

You’ve got a few options here. If the individuals are rating themselves, I think it’s reasonable to put them at the lowest score. I didn’t do that, but I think that’s justifiable (I think I tested and it didn’t matter too much in my studies to make that choice). Especially if you’re talking about ratings of others, it will be necessary to impute the median for some calculations we’ll do in a minute. You could make other choices than the median and the code is pretty similar.

The easiest way I found to do this is:

Library(hmisc)

Data_noNA <- impute_median(Data, Expertise1_A + Expertise2_A  + Expertise1_B  + Expertise2_B  + Expertise1_Self  + Expertise2_Self  ~ GroupNumber)

The ~ GroupNumber means that the group median score will be imputed for that variable. This means that if I said I didn’t know someone’s expertise, the median score that the other teammates gave that member is substituted in. Importantly, this command just fills in NA values, but once those data are filled in, they are indistinguishable from the true values so you want to be sure you don’t overwrite your original data.

Step 4: Data Crunching

Now we get into the fun stuff.

We want to add a new variable which is the standard deviation of an individual’s ratings of their own expertise. Super easy:

Data_noNA$IndSpec <- apply(Data_noNA[,X:Y],1,sd) #X:Y in this case are the columns that represent a member’s self-rating.

#Now, things get extra fun:

Data_split <- split(Data_noNA, Data_noNA$GroupNumber) #allows us to do a series of calculations within each group separately

#Now we create some empty data.frames to hold the group-level variables we’re about to create

Data_KnowStock <- as.data.frame(NA) #Our Knowledge stock variable, create as many as you need if you have more than one knowledge area.
Data_IndSpec <- as.data.frame(NA) #The individual specialization score from before. Again, if you want to calculate specialization within more than one knowledge area, create more.
Data_KnowDifferentiation <- as.data.frame(NA) #Knowledge Differentiation is a major contribution of the paper and also the reason we needed to impute NAs earlier.
DATA_UnConsensus <- as.data.frame(NA) #The major meta-knowledge variable we have here.

#Now we will loop through the split datasets

for (i in 1:Z) {  #Z is the number of groups that there are in your data
    Data_KnowStock[i,] <- mean(asNumericMatrix(lapply(Data_split[i], "[",X:Y))) #X:Y in this case are the columns that represent a member’s self-rating.
    Data_IndSpec[i,] <- mean(asNumericMatrix(lapply(Data_split[i], "[",K))) #K is the column that was added from the Individual Specialization variable we calculated earlier
    Data_KnowDifferentiation[i,] <- mean(distance_matrix(distances(as.data.frame(lapply(Data_split[i], "[",X:Y))))) #X:Y in this case are the columns that represent a member’s self-rating.
    Data_UnConsensus[i,] <- mean(distance_matrix(distances(as.data.frame(lapply(SCALE_split[i], "[",L:M))))) #L:M are the columns that represent all of the ratings of self and others (don’t double count the self ratings)
}
Data_Kush <- bind_cols(Data_KnowStock, Data_IndSpec, Data_KnowDifferentiation, Data_UnConsensus)
names(Data_Kush) <- c("KnowStock", "IndSpec", " KnowDifferentiation", "UnConsensus")
write.csv(Data_Kush,"Data_Kush.csv")

Step 4.5 Accuracy

I calculated accuracy well before I calculated some of these other variables and thus I wrote the original code in SPSS.

The accuracy variable is calculated as the sum of differences in members perceptions of each other.

Original SPSS code:

COMPUTE DifferencesInA = MEAN(ABS(Expertise1_A - Expertise1_A_Group), ABS(Expertise2_A – Expertise2_A_Group), etc…)). Execute.

Once you’ve calculated the differences for each member, you sum the these differences and then subtract from the maximum to determine accuracy.

As I stated in the paper, my goal here is to add to the toolbox in how to calculate TMS and to hopefully be able to capture the totality of its effects. If you have any questions of issues with this tutorial, please let me know as I want to make this as easy to do as possible (jkush@umassd.edu).